Living in some kind of social grouping is not
unusual among mammals and is certainly not special to primates.
Wolves, African hunting dogs and lions all exhibit complex
intra-group relationships whose sophistication may in some aspects
rival or even excel that seen in primates. Even vampire bats
demonstrate a form of social behaviour and of a type rare among
primates - the voluntary sharing of food. A vampire returning
replete with blood may regurgitate some of this liquid largesse
into the begging mouths of its less fortunate (and probably
unrelated) fellows back at the roost. Such apparently selfless
generosity to non-kin is rare among any animals and very unusual in
primates (although with the vampire bat it may not be as selfless
as it initially appears, as today's blood- giver
may be tomorrow's receiver). Even such an unlikely animal as the
naked mole-rat exhibits strangely insect-like habits. It has a
complex ant-like caste system in which the sole breeding 'queen
rat' suppresses breeding in her followers.
Primate social behaviour resembles neither that
of the vampire nor of the bizarre mole-rat. In fact in many cases
the present lack of field studies means that we are uncertain just
how the social systems of many species actually function; but
increased human presence in the field is rapidly yielding much more
information on this and other topics.
Home range
Social behaviour is based on the 'home range',
the area which is regularly used by the animal or group of animals
for all their regular activities. The home range may not be
exclusive, but may be shared with other individuals or groups of
the same and other species."Certain parts of the home range may be
used more than others - some primates only regularly use the 10 per
cent or so containing the richest food resources and favourite
sleeping sites. The borders of a home range are not defended, but
those of a 'territory' most certainly are. Territorial primates use
many methods for guarding against trespass across their boundaries
to ensure that they have exclusive use of their reserved living
space.
At its simplest, primate social behaviour may
appear at first sight to be non-social, for many of the nocturnal
prosimians appear to lead noticeably solitary lives. However, just
because a weasel lemur or bushbaby is almost always encountered
feeding alone at night does not mean that it lacks regular
communication with others of its kind. There are two main methods
of ensuring that the neighbours know who is around, while at the
same time possibly disclosing intimate details about sex and
breeding condition: by shouting loudly about it, and by leaving
smelly calling cards. The latter method is probably employed by all
nocturnal prosimians and has the advantage of staying-power. It is
also less risky, being a private communications channel
decipherable only by the noses of other members of the same species
and therefore less likely to attract the unwelcome attentions of
predators than when sitting around in an exposed spot and calling
loudly. If applied in a particular way a scent may be remarkably
enduring, as in the Cheirogaleus dwarf lemurs which use their
extruded anus as an applicator to smear a line of faeces along a
branch, leaving an odorous 'squatter's rights' proclamation capable
of sticking around for many weeks or even months. Bushbabies also
leave scent messages, but use a multiple applicator in the form of
the hands and feet. These are meticulously daubed with urine before
the animal moves off, leaving at every step a gradually declining
trail of smelly footprints which no doubt reveal a mine of
information to the recipient.
Like most (perhaps all) nocturnal prosimians,
bushbabies also use sound - up to eight different calls have been
noted in the lesser bushbaby - to communicate with their
neighbours; the calling of these diminutive animals can be one of
the most characteristic sounds of the African bush after dark.
Bushbaby society generally consists of several overlapping female
home ranges which are more or less completely covered by the much
larger home range of a single male. He makes it his frequent
business to visit each female's range to check out her breeding
condition, to be certain of being in the right place at the right
time to mate with her if she is in oestrus. Although he may not be
the only male in the vicinity, he is the largest and toughest, so
probably secures sole access to the females. Rather more
lightweight males manage to squeeze in a living between the ranges
of the females and the central 'boss' male, while other males who
have just reached adulthood lead the itinerant life of the drifter,
passing at will through the ranges of both females and central
'boss' males. The presence of even smaller immature males may also
be conceded within the range of the central male, presumably
because they do not currently present any kind of sexual threat. So
although bushbabies always seem to be alone when located in the
forest at night, they do lead busy lives which include regular
social contact with one another.
Horsefield's tarsier probably has similar
arrangements in the forests of Borneo. Adult males seem to have
larger home ranges than females, from around 8.75 to 11.25 ha (21.6
to 27.8 acres) for males and 4.5-9.5 ha (11-23.5 acres) for
females. Unlike the galagines, whose females often sleep together
during the day in a communal nest, both sexes of Horsefield's
tarsier sleep alone. In fact all the evidence suggests that this
species is perhaps the most solitary of all primates, regularly
communicating with others of its kind only through calling and
scent marking. However, there is a broad repertoire of calls, some
rather insect-like, which serve to keep close neighbours aware of
each other's presence. This may escalate to the point where at
certain times veritable concerts may be struck up by several
animals (up to five) perched in close vicinity. These concerts may
be remarkably enduring, and in one case lasted for four hours. This
concert style of calling seems to be quite different from the
alternate duets found in the spectral tarsier and in unrelated
animals such as the weasel lemur. The precise function of these
tarsier concerts is unknown, but they may be connected with
territorial boundary disputes. The exact nature of male-female
relationships in Horsefield's tarsier is currently unknown; but it
seems probable that, unlike in the galagines, two or more males may
regularly visit the range of a single female from their own ranges,
which only overlap with hers to a marginal extent. Scent marking
may not be important and vocalizations are probably the main form
of communication.